Bálint's syndrome

Bálint's syndrome
Classification and external resources
ICD-9 368.16

Bálint's syndrome is an uncommon and incompletely understood triad of severe neuropsychological impairments: inability to perceive the visual field as a whole (simultanagnosia), difficulty in fixating the eyes (ocular apraxia), and inability to move the hand to a specific object by using vision (optic ataxia).[1] It was named in 1909 for the Austro-Hungarian neurologist Rezső Bálint who first identified it.[2][3]

Bálint's syndrome occurs most often with an acute onset as a consequence of two or more strokes at more or less the same place in each hemisphere. Therefore, it occurs rarely. The most frequent cause of complete Bálint's syndrome is said by some to be sudden and severe hypotension, resulting in bilateral borderzone infarction in the occipito-parietal region.[1] More rarely, cases of progressive Bálint's syndrome have been found in degenerative disorders such as Alzheimer's disease[4] or certain other traumatic brain injuries at the border of the parietal and the occipital lobes of the brain.

Lack of awareness of this syndrome may lead to a misdiagnosis and resulting inappropriate or inadequate treatment. Therefore, clinicians should be familiar with Bálint's syndrome and its various etiologies.[5]

Contents

Symptoms

Bálint's syndrome symptoms can be quite debilitating since they impact visuospatial skills, visual scanning and attentional mechanisms.[6] Since it represents impairment of both visual and language functions, it is a significant disability that can affect the patient's safety─even in one's own home environment, and can render the person incapable of maintaining employment.[7] In many cases the complete trio of symptoms—inability to perceive the visual field as a whole (simultanagnosia), difficulty in fixating the eyes (ocular apraxia), and inability to move the hand to a specific object by using vision (optic ataxia)—may not be noticed until the patient is in rehabilitation. Therapists unfamiliar with Bálint's syndrome may misdiagnose a patient's inability to meet progress expectations in any of these symptom areas as simply indicating incapability of benefiting from further traditional therapy. The very nature of each Bálint symptom frustrates rehabilitation progress in each of the other symptoms. Much more research is needed to develop therapeutic protocols that address Bálint symptoms as a group since the disabilities are so intertwined.

Simultanagnosia

Simultanagnosia is the inability to perceive simultaneous events or objects in one's visual field.[8] Victims of Bálint's syndrome perceive the world erratically, as a series of single objects rather than seeing the wholeness of a scene.[9]

This spatial disorder of visual attention—the ability to identify local elements of a scene, but not the global whole—has been referred to as a constriction of the individual's global gestalt window—their visual "window" of attention. People fixate their eyes to specific images in social scenes because they are informative to the meaning of the scene. Any forthcoming recovery in simultanagnosia may be related to somehow expanding the restricted attentional window that characterizes this disorder.[10]

Simultanagnosia is a profound visual deficit. It impairs the ability to perceive multiple items in a visual display, while preserving the ability to recognize single objects. One study suggests that simultanagnosia may result from an extreme form of competition between objects which makes it difficult for attention to be disengaged from an object once it has been selected.[11] Patients with simultanagnosia have a restricted spatial window of visual attention and cannot see more than one object at a time. They see their world in a patchy, spotty manner. Therefore, they pick out a single object, or even components of an individual object, without being able to see the global "big picture."

A study which directly tested the relationship between the restriction of the attentional window in simultanagnosia compared with the vision of healthy participants with normal limits of visual processing confirmed the limitations of difficulties of patients with simultanagnosia.[12]

There is considerable evidence that a person's cortex is essentially divided into two functional streams: an occipital-parietal-frontal pathway that processes "where" information and an occipital-temporal-frontal pathway that provides "what" information to the individual.[13]

Ocular apraxia

Ocular apraxia is exemplified by the inability to carry out familiar movements when asked to do so. Persons with apraxia understand such commands and are willing to carry them out. However, they are physically unable to perform the task.[14]

Apraxia of speech, also known as verbal apraxia or dyspraxia, is a speech disorder in which a person has trouble saying what he or she wants to say correctly and consistently. Apraxia of speech is often present along with another speech disorder called aphasia. At times, a completely different word or action is used than the one the person intended to speak or make. The person is often aware of the mistake.[15]

Bálint referred to this as "psychic paralysis of gaze"—the inability to voluntarily guide eye movements, changing to a new location of visual fixation. The most frequent and disabling deficit is the syndrome of unilateral spatial neglect that manifests as a bias of spatial representation and attention on the same side as the lesion. Also significant is that the disturbances of space representation are more frequent and severe after traumatic damage to the right hemisphere.[16]

There is no single factor or test that can be used to diagnose apraxia. In addition, speech-language experts do not agree about which specific symptoms are part of developmental apraxia.

In some cases, people with acquired apraxia of speech recover some or all of their speech abilities on their own. In other cases, speech-language therapy, tailored to the individual and designed to treat other speech or language problems that may occur together with apraxia, is often helpful.[15]

Optic ataxia

Optic ataxia is the inability to guide the hand toward an object using visual information[17] where the inability cannot be explained by motor, somatosensory, visual field deficits or acuity deficits.[18] Optic ataxia is seen in Bálint's syndrome where it is characterized by an impaired visual control of the direction of arm-reaching to a visual target, accompanied by defective hand orientation and grip formation.[19] It is considered a specific visuomotor disorder, independent of visual space misperception.[18]

Optic ataxia is also known as misreaching or dysmetria (English: difficult to measure), secondary to visual perceptual deficits. A patient with Bálint's syndrome likely has defective hand movements under visual guidance, despite normal limb strength. The patient is unable to grab an object while looking at the object, due to a discoordination of eye and hand movement. It is especially true with their contralesional hand.[18]

Dysmetria refers to a lack of coordination of movement, typified by the undershoot or overshoot of intended position with the hand, arm, leg, or eye. It is sometimes described as an inability to judge distance or scale.[20]

As Bálint states, optic ataxia impaired his patient’s daily activities, since, ‘while cutting a slice of meat...which he held with a fork in his left hand, ...would search for it outside the plate with the knife in his right hand’, or ‘...while lighting a cigarette he often lit the middle and not the end’. Bálint pointed out the systematic nature of this disorder, which was evident in the patient’s behaviour when searching in space. ‘Thus, when asked to grasp a presented object with his right hand, he would miss it regularly, and would find it only when his hand knocked against it.[19]

The reaching ability of the patient is also altered. It takes them longer to reach toward an object. Their ability to grasp an object is also impaired. The patient's performance is even more severely deteriorated when vision of either the hand or the target is prevented.[18]

Cause

The visual difficulties in Bálint's syndrome are usually due to damage to the top part of the temporal-occipital lobes on both sides of the brain. The temporal lobe is on the side of the brain by the ear and the occipital lobe is the back part of the brain. Thus, the temporal-occipital lobes refer to the sides and back parts of the brain. In Balint's syndrome, the top part of the parietal lobes on both sides of the brain may also be affected. The parietal lobes are the middle area of the top part of the brain.

Diagnosis

Lack of awareness of the syndrome may lead to misdiagnosis such as blindness, psychosis, or dementia.[1] Symptoms of Bálint's syndrome are most likely to be noticed first by therapists providing rehabilitation following brain lesions. However, due to the scarcity among practitioners of familiarity with the syndrome, the symptoms are often explained away incorrectly without being considered as a possibility and followed by medical confirmation of clinical and neuroradiological findings.[21] Any severe disturbance of space representation, spontaneously appearing following bilateral parietal damage, strongly suggests the presence of Bálint's syndrome and should be investigated as such.[22] One study reports that damage to the bilateral dorsal occipitoparietal regions appeared to be involved in Bálint's syndrome.[23]

Neuroanatomical evidence

Bálint's syndrome has been found in patients with bilateral damage to the posterior parietal cortex. The primary cause of the damage and the syndrome can originate from multiple strokes, Alzheimer's disease, intracranial tumors, or brain injury. Multifocal leukoencephalopathy and Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease have also been found to cause this kind of damage. This syndrome is caused by damage to the posterior superior watershed areas, also known as the parietal-occipital vascular border zone (Brodmann's areas 19 and 7).

Manifestations

Some telltale signs suggesting Bálint's syndrome following bilateral brain insults may include:

Treatment

In terms of the specific rehabilitation of visuoperceptual disorders such as Bálint’s syndrome, the literature is extremely sparse.[6] According to one study, rehabilitation training should focus on the improvement of visual scanning, the development of visually guided manual movements, and the improvement of the integration of visual elements.[24] Very few treatment strategies have been proposed, and some of those have been criticized as being poorly developed and evaluated.

Three approaches to rehabilitation of perceptual deficits, such as those seen in Bálint's syndrome, have been identified:

  1. The adaptive (functional) approach, which involves functional tasks utilising the person’s strengths and abilities, helping them to compensate for problems or altering the environment to lessen their disabilities. This is the most popular approach.
  2. The remedial approach, which involves restoration of the damaged CNS by training in the perceptual skills, which may be generalised across all activities of daily living. This could be achieved by tabletop activities or sensorimotor exercises.
  3. The multicontext approach, which is based on the fact that learning is not automatically transferred from one situation to another. This involves practicing of a targeted strategy in a multiple environment with varied tasks and movement demands, and it incorporates self-awareness tasks.[25]

The Web site "Medfriendly" is one of the few sites found to contain structured therapeutic techniques that provide the patient with immediate feedback.

  1. Helping the patient focus on more than one thing at a time and to improve the ability to tell things apart with vision. In this technique, flash cards are shown to the patient with two lines on them. The patient is asked if the two lines look the same or different. The answer is written on the back of the flash card. If the angles are different, the degree of difference is noted on the back of the card as well. By using this technique, the patient can get immediate feedback as to whether he/she perceives things accurately. Also, a family member can use the flash cards with the patient or the patient can use them by him or herself.[26]
  2. Different types of flashcards can also be used to help the patient focus on the general aspect of something instead of only focusing on the details. The flashcards have a clock face on the front with all 12 numbers. Once the patient is used to this, the clocks only have the numbers 12, 3, 6, and 9 on them. Finally, the numbers are faded out and the patient is asked to tell what time it is based only on the hands of the clock. This requires focusing on the details to answer a general question based on visual information. The time is written in digital form on the back of the cards so the patient can get immediate feedback.[26]
  3. Some patient's with Balint's syndrome only respond to part of what they see when the object(s) is close up. Thus, one strategy is to determine how far the patient can see and to place the object(s) farther away in the field of vision. In some cases, this helps the patient improve the ability to see the entire object and not just a part of it.[26]
  4. Since people with Balint's syndrome cannot look into the sides of their visual field, techniques can be used to improve the person's ability to scan visual patterns quickly. The visual field is the space that someone can see without moving the eyes. Some techniques to improve visual ability include having the patient make wide head turns towards the areas that he/she cannot see when looking straight ahead. Such wide head turns help the patient gather more information about the environment.[26]
  5. Reading material that is in the area that the person has difficulty seeing can be marked with an up and down red line. By doing this, the patient will see the red line and be more aware of their difficulty attending to that area of the page. This, in turn, can allow the patient to be sure to focus more attention on that particular area of the page.[26]
  6. To improve optic ataxia (inability to move the hand to an object by using vision), exercises can be done that require eye/hand coordination and manipulating objects with the hands. Using tools would be an example as would be catching a ball. Obviously, safe tools should be used and it is often helpful to start the ball-catching exercise with a ball made of crunched up paper. The patient can then progress to balls that travel faster when thrown (e.g., tennis balls). This task can be made more difficult by bouncing the ball against a wall first.[26]

Some case studies

Symptoms of Bálint’s syndrome were found in the case of a 29-year-old migraine sufferer. In the aura before the migraine headache, she experienced an inability to see all of the objects in the visual field simultaneously; an inability to coordinate hand and eye movements; and an inability to look at an object on command.[27] Symptoms were not present before the onset of the migraine or after it passed.

A study of a patient with Corticobasal Ganglionic Degeneration (CGBD) also showed a development of Bálint’s syndrome. As a result of CGBD, the patient developed an inability to move his eyes to specific visual objects in his peripheral fields. He also was unable to reach out and touch objects in his peripheral fields. An inability to recognize more than one item at a time was also experienced when presented with the Cookie Theft Picture[28] from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination.

A 58-year old male presented with Bálint's syndrome secondary to severe traumatic brain injury 4-months post-injury onset. He had completed a comprehensive post-acute brain injury rehabilitation program. He received 6 months of rehabilitation services as an inpatient. A three-pronged approach included the implementation of (a) compensatory strategies, (b) remediation exercises and (c) transfer of learned skills in multiple environments and situations. Comprehensive neuropsychological and occupational therapy evaluations were performed at admission and at discharge. Neuropsychological test improvements were noted on tasks that assess visuospatial functioning, although most gains were noted for functional and physical abilities.[6]

A patient with congenital deafness exhibited partial Bálint’s syndrome symptoms. This patient experienced an inability to perceive simultaneous events in her visual field. She was also unable to fixate and follow an object with her eyes. In addition, her ability to point at targets under visual guidance was impaired.[21]

Bálint's syndrome is rarely reported in children, but some recent studies provide evidence that cases do exist in children. A case involving a 10-year-old male child with Bálint's syndrome has been reported.[29] Similar results were seen in a 7-year-old boy. In children this syndrome results in a variety of occupational difficulties, but most notably difficulties in schoolwork, especially reading. The investigators encourage more careful recognition of the syndrome to allow adequate rehabilitation and environmental adaptation.[30]

Criticism

The validity of Bálint's syndrome has been questioned by some. The components in the syndrome's triad of defects (simultanagnosia, ocular apraxia, optic ataxia) each may represent a variety of combined defects.[31]

Because Bálint's syndrome is not common and is difficult to assess with standard clinical tools, the literature is dominated by case reports and confounded by case selection bias, non-uniform application of operational definitions, inadequate study of basic vision, poor lesion localisation, and failure to distinguish between deficits in the acute and chronic phases of recovery.[9]

References

  1. ^ a b c <Udesen H, Madsen AL. "Bálint's syndrome—visual disorientation." Ugeskr Laeger. 1992 May 18;154(21):1492-4. [1]
  2. ^ synd/1343 at Who Named It?
  3. ^ R. Bálint. Seelenlähmung des "Schauens", optische Ataxia, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie, 1909, 25: 51-81.
  4. ^ P. Ribaia, M. Vokaer, et al. "Acute Bálint’s syndrome is not always caused by a stroke." European Journal of Neurology; Mar2006, Vol. 13 Issue 3, p310-312
  5. ^ Perez FM, Tunkel RS, Lachmann EA, Nagler W. "Bálint's syndrome arising from bilateral posterior cortical atrophy or infarction: rehabilitation strategies and their limitation." Disabil Rehabil. 1996 Jun;18(6):300-4.
  6. ^ a b c Zgaljardic DJ, Yancy S, Levinson J, Morales G, Masel BE. "Bálint's syndrome and post-acute brain injury rehabilitation: A case report." Brain Inj. 2011;25(9):909-17. Epub 2011 Jun 1.
  7. ^ Toyokura, M., & Koike, T. (2006). "Rehabilitative intervention and social participation of a case with Bálintʼs syndrome and aphasia." Tokai Journal of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, 31(2), 78-82. Retrieved from [2]
  8. ^ "Bálint Syndrome and Related Visuo-Spatial and Visuo-Motor Disturbances". Medscape News Today. http://www.medscape.com/viewarticle/410860_5
  9. ^ a b Rizzo M, Vecera SP. "Psychoanatomical substrates of Bálint's syndrome." J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002 Feb;72(2):162-78.
  10. ^ Dalrymple K A, Birmingham E, et al. "Experiencing simultanagnosia through windowed viewing of complex social scenes." Brain Res. 2011 Jan 7;1367:265-77.
  11. ^ Jackson GM; Swainson R. "Attention, competition, and the parietal lobes: insights from Bálint's syndrome." Psychological Research 2009 Mar; Vol. 73 (2), pp. 263-70
  12. ^ Dalrymple K A, Bischof W F, et al. "Simulating simultanagnosia spatially constricted vision mimics local capture and the global processing deficit." Brain Res. 201 Apr; 202(2) 445-455
  13. ^ Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982. Reported in Kim MS, Robertson LC. "Implicit representations of space after bilateral parietal lobe damage." J Cogn Neeurosci. 2001 Nov 15;13(8):1080-7
  14. ^ Cleveland Clinic. Diseases and conditions
  15. ^ a b "Apraxia of Speech." National Institutes of Health. Apraxia of Speech
  16. ^ Vallar, G. "Spatial neglect, Bálint-Homes' and Gerstmann's syndrome, and other spatial disorders." CNS Spectr. 2007 Jul;12(7):527-36
  17. ^ Perenin, M. and A. Vighetto. “Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms.” ‘’Brain’’ (1988) 111(3): 643-674 doi:10.1093/brain/111.3.643
  18. ^ a b c d e Radtke, Susan and Sarah Leonard. "Optic ataxia." U of Waterloo
  19. ^ a b Battaglia‐Mayer, Alexandra and Roberto Caminiti. "Optic ataxia as a result of the breakdown of the global tuning fields of parietal neurones".Brain (2002) 125 (2): 225-237. <http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/125/2/225.full>
  20. ^ “Dysmetria,” Multiple Sclerosis Encyclopaedia, Last modified 16 October 2007, http://www.mult-sclerosis.org/dysmetria.html/
  21. ^ a b Drane DL; Lee GP, et al. "Development of a partial Bálint's syndrome in a congenitally deaf patient presenting as pseudo-aphasia." Clin Neuropsychol. 2009 May;23(4):715-28. Epub 2008 Oct 16.
  22. ^ Valenza N, Murray MM, Ptak R, Vuilleumier P. "The space of senses: impaired cross modal interactionsin a patient with Bálint syndrome after bilateral parietal damage." Neuropsychologia." 2004; 42(13)"1737-48
  23. ^ Kas A, de Souza LC, Samri D, et al. "Neural correlates of cognitive impairment in posterior cortical atrophy." Brain. 2011 May;134(Pt 5):1464-78. Epub 2011 Apr 7.
  24. ^ Rosselli M; Ardila A; Beltran C. "Rehabilitation of Bálint's syndrome." Applied Neuropsychology [Appl Neuropsychol] 2001; Vol. 8 (4), pp. 242-7.
  25. ^ Al-Khawaja, I. and N. H. J. Haboubi. Al-Khawaja. (2001) "Neurovisual rehabilitation in Bálint’s syndrome." J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;70:406–425 [3] and [4]
  26. ^ a b c d e f g <http://www.medfriendly.com/balintssyndrome.php5>
  27. ^ Shah, P. & Nafee, A. (1999) Migraine aura masquerading as Bálint’s syndrome. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 67:554-555. Found at http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/picrender.fcgi?artid=1736566&blobtype=pdf
  28. ^ Mendez, Mario F., M.D., Ph.D. (2000). "Corticobasal Ganglionic Degeneration With Bálint’s Syndrome". The Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience. Found at http://neuro.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/content/full/12/2/273
  29. ^ Gillen, J.A. and Dutton, G.N. (2003) Bálint's syndrome in a 10-year-old male. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 45:349-32.
  30. ^ Drummond SR, Dutton GN. "Simultanagnosia." J AAPOS. 2007 Oct;11(5):497=8.
  31. ^ Rizzo M, Smith V, Pokorny J, et al. "Color perception profiles in central achromatopsia." Neurology 1993;43:995-1001